Photo report

Recherches sur la cognition animale au CRCA

Recherches sur la cognition animale au CRCA

20170048_0001
153 media
20170048_0001
Open media modal

Labelling mouse brain sections via immunohistochemistry (IHC). The baths in which these sections are immersed contain antibodies that make it possible, in the experiment, to locate doublecortin proteins, which are markers of immature neurons. The study focuses on adult neurogenesis, i.e. the development of neurons during adulthood, in the hippocampus. The sections are then placed in baths to wash them, and the proteins are detected via fluorescence or visible light. The brains used are from…

Photo
20170048_0001
Marquage par immunohistochimie de coupes de cerveaux de souris
20170048_0002
Open media modal

Labelling mouse brain sections via immunohistochemistry (IHC). The baths in which these sections are immersed contain antibodies that make it possible, in the experiment, to locate doublecortin proteins, which are markers of immature neurons. The study focuses on adult neurogenesis, i.e. the development of neurons during adulthood, in the hippocampus. The sections are then placed in baths to wash them, and the proteins are detected via fluorescence or visible light. The brains used are from…

Photo
20170048_0002
Marquage par immunohistochimie de coupes de cerveaux de souris
20170048_0003
Open media modal

Labelling mouse brain sections via immunohistochemistry (IHC). The baths in which these sections are immersed contain antibodies that make it possible, in the experiment, to locate doublecortin proteins, which are markers of immature neurons. The study focuses on adult neurogenesis, i.e. the development of neurons during adulthood, in the hippocampus. The sections are then placed in baths to wash them, and the proteins are detected via fluorescence or visible light. The brains used are from…

Photo
20170048_0003
Marquage par immunohistochimie de coupes de cerveaux de souris
20170048_0004
Open media modal

Labelling mouse brain sections via immunohistochemistry (IHC). The baths in which these sections are immersed contain antibodies that make it possible, in the experiment, to locate doublecortin proteins, which are markers of immature neurons. The study focuses on adult neurogenesis, i.e. the development of neurons during adulthood, in the hippocampus. The sections are then placed in baths to wash them, and the proteins are detected via fluorescence or visible light. The brains used are from…

Photo
20170048_0004
Marquage par immunohistochimie de coupes de cerveaux de souris
20170048_0005
Open media modal

Labelling mouse brain sections via immunohistochemistry (IHC). The baths in which these sections are immersed contain antibodies that make it possible, in the experiment, to locate doublecortin proteins, which are markers of immature neurons. The study focuses on adult neurogenesis, i.e. the development of neurons during adulthood, in the hippocampus. The sections are then placed in baths to wash them, and the proteins are detected via fluorescence or visible light. The brains used are from…

Photo
20170048_0005
Marquage par immunohistochimie de coupes de cerveaux de souris
20170048_0006
Open media modal

Labelling mouse brain sections via immunohistochemistry (IHC). The baths in which these sections are immersed contain antibodies that make it possible, in the experiment, to locate doublecortin proteins, which are markers of immature neurons. The study focuses on adult neurogenesis, i.e. the development of neurons during adulthood, in the hippocampus. The sections are then placed in baths to wash them, and the proteins are detected via fluorescence or visible light. The brains used are from…

Photo
20170048_0006
Marquage par immunohistochimie de coupes de cerveaux de souris
20170048_0007
Open media modal

Mounting brain sections on slides after labelling via immunohistochemistry (IHC), for viewing through a microscope. These sections have been immersed in baths containing antibodies that make it possible, in the experiment, to locate doublecortin proteins, which are markers of immature neurons. The study focuses on adult neurogenesis, i.e. the development of neurons during adulthood, in the hippocampus. The sections are then immersed in a bath that allows the detection of proteins via…

Photo
20170048_0007
Montage de coupes de cerveaux de souris sur des lames, après marquage par immunohistochimie
20170048_0008
Open media modal

Mounting brain sections on slides after labelling via immunohistochemistry (IHC), for viewing through a microscope. These sections have been immersed in baths containing antibodies that make it possible, in the experiment, to locate doublecortin proteins, which are markers of immature neurons. The study focuses on adult neurogenesis, i.e. the development of neurons during adulthood, in the hippocampus. The sections are then immersed in a bath that allows the detection of proteins via…

Photo
20170048_0008
Montage de coupes de cerveaux de souris sur des lames, après marquage par immunohistochimie
20170048_0009
Open media modal

Mounting brain sections on slides after labelling via immunohistochemistry (IHC), for viewing through a microscope. These sections have been immersed in baths containing antibodies that make it possible, in the experiment, to locate doublecortin proteins, which are markers of immature neurons. The study focuses on adult neurogenesis, i.e. the development of neurons during adulthood, in the hippocampus. The sections are then immersed in a bath that allows the detection of proteins via…

Photo
20170048_0009
Montage de coupes de cerveaux de souris sur des lames, après marquage par immunohistochimie
20170048_0010
Open media modal

Séchage de coupes de cerveaux colorées et disposées sur des lames après marquage par immunohistochimie (IHC). Les bains de coloration rose permettent l'apparition d'une contre coloration marquant les noyaux des cellules, facilitent l’identification des aires du cerveau et des zones d’accumulation. Deux autres bains sont effectués, un à forte concentration en alcool déshydrate les coupes, l’autre composé d’un solvant organique, les clarifie en éliminant les lipides. Ces coupes ont été…

Photo
20170048_0010
Séchage de coupes de cerveaux colorées disposées sur des lames après marquage par immunohistochimie
20170048_0011
Open media modal

Observation au microscope à champ large de lames de coupes de cerveaux de souris après marquage par immunohistochimie (IHC). La chercheuse compte les cellules marquées, soit les cellules immatures. La coloration rose permet l'apparition d'une contre coloration marquant les noyaux de cellules, facilitant l’identification des aires du cerveau et des zones d’accumulation. Ce microscope permet de faire des observations en fluorescence ou en méthode visible. Les coupes observées ont été…

Photo
20170048_0011
Observation au microscope de coupes de cerveaux de souris après marquage par immunohistochimie
20170048_0012
Open media modal

Observation au microscope à champ large de lames de coupes de cerveaux de souris après marquage par immunohistochimie (IHC). La chercheuse compte les cellules marquées, soit les cellules immatures. La coloration rose permet l'apparition d'une contre coloration marquant les noyaux de cellules, facilitant l’identification des aires du cerveau et des zones d’accumulation. Ce microscope permet de faire des observations en fluorescence ou en méthode visible. Les coupes observées ont été…

Photo
20170048_0012
Observation au microscope de coupes de cerveaux de souris après marquage par immunohistochimie
20170048_0013
Open media modal

Observation au microscope à champ large de lames de coupes de cerveaux de souris après marquage par immunohistochimie (IHC). La chercheuse compte les cellules marquées, soit les cellules immatures. La coloration rose permet l'apparition d'une contre coloration marquant les noyaux de cellules, facilitant l’identification des aires du cerveau et des zones d’accumulation. Ce microscope permet de faire des observations en fluorescence ou en méthode visible. Les coupes observées ont été…

Photo
20170048_0013
Observation au microscope de coupes de cerveaux de souris après marquage par immunohistochimie
20170048_0014
Open media modal

Coloration de coupes de cerveaux de souris disposées sur des lames après marquage par immunohistochimie (IHC). Les bains de coloration rose permettent l’apparition d’une contre coloration marquant les noyaux de cellules, facilitent l’identification des aires du cerveau et des zones d’accumulation. Deux autres bains sont effectués : un à forte concentration en alcool déshydrate les coupes, l’autre, composé d’un solvant organique, les clarifie en éliminant les lipides. Ces coupes ont été…

Photo
20170048_0014
Coloration de coupes de cerveaux de souris disposées sur des lames marquage par immunohistochimie
20170048_0015
Open media modal

Coloration de coupes de cerveaux de souris disposées sur des lames après marquage par immunohistochimie (IHC). Les bains de coloration rose permettent l’apparition d’une contre coloration marquant les noyaux de cellules, facilitent l’identification des aires du cerveau et des zones d’accumulation. Deux autres bains sont effectués : un à forte concentration en alcool déshydrate les coupes, l’autre, composé d’un solvant organique, les clarifie en éliminant les lipides. Ces coupes ont été…

Photo
20170048_0015
Coloration de coupes de cerveaux de souris disposées sur des lames marquage par immunohistochimie
20170048_0016
Open media modal

Coloration de coupes de cerveaux de souris disposées sur des lames après marquage par immunohistochimie (IHC). Les bains de coloration rose permettent l’apparition d’une contre coloration marquant les noyaux de cellules, facilitent l’identification des aires du cerveau et des zones d’accumulation. Deux autres bains sont effectués : un à forte concentration en alcool déshydrate les coupes, l’autre, composé d’un solvant organique, les clarifie en éliminant les lipides. Ces coupes ont été…

Photo
20170048_0016
Coloration de coupes de cerveaux de souris disposées sur des lames marquage par immunohistochimie
20170048_0017
Open media modal

Coloration de coupes de cerveaux de souris disposées sur des lames après marquage par immunohistochimie (IHC). Les bains de coloration rose permettent l’apparition d’une contre coloration marquant les noyaux de cellules, facilitent l’identification des aires du cerveau et des zones d’accumulation. Deux autres bains sont effectués : un à forte concentration en alcool déshydrate les coupes, l’autre, composé d’un solvant organique, les clarifie en éliminant les lipides. Ces coupes ont été…

Photo
20170048_0017
Coloration de coupes de cerveaux de souris disposées sur des lames marquage par immunohistochimie
20170048_0018
Open media modal

Coloration de coupes de cerveaux de souris disposées sur des lames après marquage par immunohistochimie (IHC). Les bains de coloration rose permettent l’apparition d’une contre coloration marquant les noyaux de cellules, facilitent l’identification des aires du cerveau et des zones d’accumulation. Deux autres bains sont effectués : un à forte concentration en alcool déshydrate les coupes, l’autre, composé d’un solvant organique, les clarifie en éliminant les lipides. Ces coupes ont été…

Photo
20170048_0018
Coloration de coupes de cerveaux de souris disposées sur des lames marquage par immunohistochimie
20170048_0019
Open media modal

Coloration de coupes de cerveaux de souris disposées sur des lames après marquage par immunohistochimie (IHC). Les bains de coloration rose permettent l’apparition d’une contre coloration marquant les noyaux de cellules, facilitent l’identification des aires du cerveau et des zones d’accumulation. Deux autres bains sont effectués : un à forte concentration en alcool déshydrate les coupes, l’autre, composé d’un solvant organique, les clarifie en éliminant les lipides. Ces coupes ont été…

Photo
20170048_0019
Coloration de coupes de cerveaux de souris disposées sur des lames marquage par immunohistochimie
20170048_0021
Open media modal

Mouse being placed in an arena designed to test its spatial memory. The test is based on the capacity of mice to remember the location of objects present in the arena. The first stage involves placing the mouse in the arena with two identical objects so it can familiarise itself with them and learn their position. A striped visual pattern helps the mouse orient itself in the space. To avoid placing the animal under stress, the test environment does not receive direct light. After this first…

Photo
20170048_0021
Une souris est déposée dans un test de mémoire spatiale
20170048_0022
Open media modal

Mouse exploring its environment in an arena designed to test its spatial memory. The test is based on the capacity of mice to remember the location of objects present in the arena. The first stage involves placing the individual in the arena with two identical objects so it can familiarise itself with them and learn their position. A striped visual pattern helps the mouse orient itself in the space. To avoid placing the animal under stress, the test environment does not receive direct light…

Photo
20170048_0022
Une souris explore l’environnement d’un dispositif de test de mémoire spatiale
20170048_0023
Open media modal

Mouse exploring its environment in an arena designed to test its spatial memory. The test is based on the capacity of mice to remember the location of objects present in the arena. The first stage involves placing the individual in the arena with two identical objects so it can familiarise itself with them and learn their position. A striped visual pattern helps the mouse orient itself in the space. To avoid placing the animal under stress, the test environment does not receive direct light…

Photo
20170048_0023
Une souris explore l’environnement d’un dispositif de test de mémoire spatiale
20170048_0024
Open media modal

Analysis of mouse movements in a spatial memory testing arena using trajectory analysis software. The movements and behaviour of the animal in the arena are monitored via camera and analysed using special software. During the test, the mouse is first placed in the arena with two identical objects so it can familiarise itself with them and learn their position. A striped visual pattern helps the mouse orient itself in the space, and the test environment does not receive direct light so as to…

Photo
20170048_0024
Analyse des déplacements d’une souris dans un dispositif de test de la mémoire spatiale
20170048_0030
Open media modal

Mouse exploring the open arm of an elevated plus maze. This test can be used to measure anxiety levels in animals. It is based on the natural preference of rodents for closed spaces, which are more reassuring, in comparison to open spaces, which increase anxiety. The less anxious the mouse, the more likely it is to explore the open arms of the maze. By measuring the time spent in the open arms and the closed arms, it is possible to compare the anxiety levels of different mouse lines and…

Photo
20170048_0030
Une souris explore le bras ouvert d’un labyrinthe en croix
20170048_0031
Open media modal

Mouse at the centre of a plus maze during an exploratory phase. This test can be used to measure anxiety levels in animals. It is based on the natural preference of rodents for closed spaces, which are more reassuring, in comparison to open spaces, which increase anxiety. The less anxious the mouse, the more likely it is to explore the open arms of the maze. By measuring the time spent in the open arms and the closed arms, it is possible to compare the anxiety levels of different mouse lines…

Photo
20170048_0031
Une souris en phase exploratoire au centre d’un labyrinthe en croix
20170048_0032
Open media modal

Mouse at the centre of a plus maze during an exploratory phase. This test can be used to measure anxiety levels in animals. It is based on the natural preference of rodents for closed spaces, which are more reassuring, in comparison to open spaces, which increase anxiety. The less anxious the mouse, the more likely it is to explore the open arms of the maze. By measuring the time spent in the open arms and the closed arms, it is possible to compare the anxiety levels of different mouse lines…

Photo
20170048_0032
Une souris en phase exploratoire au centre d’un labyrinthe en croix
20170048_0033
Open media modal

Mouse at the centre of a plus maze during an exploratory phase. This test can be used to measure anxiety levels in animals. It is based on the natural preference of rodents for closed spaces, which are more reassuring, in comparison to open spaces, which increase anxiety. The less anxious the mouse, the more likely it is to explore the open arms of the maze. By measuring the time spent in the open arms and the closed arms, it is possible to compare the anxiety levels of different mouse lines…

Photo
20170048_0033
Une souris en phase exploratoire au centre d’un labyrinthe en croix
20170048_0034
Open media modal

Mouse exploring the open arm of an elevated plus maze. This test can be used to measure anxiety levels in animals. It is based on the natural preference of rodents for closed spaces, which are more reassuring, in comparison to open spaces, which increase anxiety. The less anxious the mouse, the more likely it is to explore the open arms of the maze. By measuring the time spent in the open arms and the closed arms, it is possible to compare the anxiety levels of different mouse lines and…

Photo
20170048_0034
Une souris explore le bras ouvert d’un labyrinthe en croix
20170048_0035
Open media modal

Mouse exploring the open arm of an elevated plus maze. This test can be used to measure anxiety levels in animals. It is based on the natural preference of rodents for closed spaces, which are more reassuring, in comparison to open spaces, which increase anxiety. The less anxious the mouse, the more likely it is to explore the open arms of the maze. By measuring the time spent in the open arms and the closed arms, it is possible to compare the anxiety levels of different mouse lines and…

Photo
20170048_0035
Une souris explore le bras ouvert d’un labyrinthe en croix
20170048_0036
Open media modal

Mouse being placed in a Barnes maze in order to test its spatial memory. The maze has 20 holes, with only one leading to a refuge. The mouse is first placed in the middle of the maze using a beaker. It will quickly try to shelter due to its aversion to the brightly lit environment. The mouse completes several sessions to test its capacity to find the refuge identified the first time again. After the learning phase, the position of the exit hole may be changed to evaluate the animal’s capacity…

Photo
20170048_0036
Souris déposée dans un labyrinthe de Barnes pour un test de mémoire spatiale complexe
20170048_0037
Open media modal

Mouse being placed in a Barnes maze in order to test its spatial memory. The maze has 20 holes, with only one leading to a refuge. The mouse is first placed in the middle of the maze using a beaker. It will quickly try to shelter due to its aversion to the brightly lit environment. The mouse completes several sessions to test its capacity to find the refuge identified the first time again. After the learning phase, the position of the exit hole may be changed to evaluate the animal’s capacity…

Photo
20170048_0037
Souris déposée dans un labyrinthe de Barnes pour un test de mémoire spatiale complexe
20170048_0038
Open media modal

Mouse being placed in a Barnes maze in order to test its spatial memory. The maze has 20 holes, with only one leading to a refuge. The mouse is first placed in the middle of the maze using a beaker. It will quickly try to shelter due to its aversion to the brightly lit environment. The mouse completes several sessions to test its capacity to find the refuge identified the first time again. After the learning phase, the position of the exit hole may be changed to evaluate the animal’s capacity…

Photo
20170048_0038
Souris déposée dans un labyrinthe de Barnes pour un test de mémoire spatiale complexe
20170048_0039
Open media modal

Mouse during an exploratory phase in a Barnes maze. The maze has 20 holes, with only one leading to a refuge. The mouse is first placed in the middle of the maze using a beaker. It will quickly try to shelter due to its aversion to the brightly lit environment. The mouse completes several sessions to test its capacity to find the refuge identified the first time again. After the learning phase, the position of the exit hole may be changed to evaluate the animal’s capacity to take on board a…

Photo
20170048_0039
Souris sur un labyrinthe de Barnes pour un test de mémoire spatiale complexe
20170048_0040
Open media modal

Mouse during an exploratory phase in a Barnes maze. The maze has 20 holes, with only one leading to a refuge. The mouse is first placed in the middle of the maze using a beaker. It will quickly try to shelter due to its aversion to the brightly lit environment. The mouse completes several sessions to test its capacity to find the refuge identified the first time again. After the learning phase, the position of the exit hole may be changed to evaluate the animal’s capacity to take on board a…

Photo
20170048_0040
Souris sur un labyrinthe de Barnes pour un test de mémoire spatiale complexe
20170048_0041
Open media modal

Mouse during an exploratory phase in a Barnes maze. The maze has 20 holes, with only one leading to a refuge. The mouse is first placed in the middle of the maze using a beaker. It will quickly try to shelter due to its aversion to the brightly lit environment. The mouse completes several sessions to test its capacity to find the refuge identified the first time again. After the learning phase, the position of the exit hole may be changed to evaluate the animal’s capacity to take on board a…

Photo
20170048_0041
Souris sur un labyrinthe de Barnes pour un test de mémoire spatiale complexe
20170048_0042
Open media modal

Mouse during an exploratory phase in a Barnes maze. The maze has 20 holes, with only one leading to a refuge. The mouse is first placed in the middle of the maze using a beaker. It will quickly try to shelter due to its aversion to the brightly lit environment. The mouse completes several sessions to test its capacity to find the refuge identified the first time again. After the learning phase, the position of the exit hole may be changed to evaluate the animal’s capacity to take on board a…

Photo
20170048_0042
Souris sur un labyrinthe de Barnes pour un test de mémoire spatiale complexe
20170048_0043
Open media modal

Mouse during an exploratory phase in a Barnes maze. The maze has 20 holes, with only one leading to a refuge. The mouse is first placed in the middle of the maze using a beaker. It will quickly try to shelter due to its aversion to the brightly lit environment. The mouse completes several sessions to test its capacity to find the refuge identified the first time again. After the learning phase, the position of the exit hole may be changed to evaluate the animal’s capacity to take on board a…

Photo
20170048_0043
Souris sur un labyrinthe de Barnes pour un test de mémoire spatiale complexe
20170048_0044
Open media modal

Mouse during an exploratory phase in a Barnes maze. The maze has 20 holes, with only one leading to a refuge. The mouse is first placed in the middle of the maze using a beaker. It will quickly try to shelter due to its aversion to the brightly lit environment. The mouse completes several sessions to test its capacity to find the refuge identified the first time again. After the learning phase, the position of the exit hole may be changed to evaluate the animal’s capacity to take on board a…

Photo
20170048_0044
Souris sur un labyrinthe de Barnes pour un test de mémoire spatiale complexe
20170048_0045
Open media modal

Mouse during an exploratory phase in a Barnes maze. The maze has 20 holes, with only one leading to a refuge. The mouse is first placed in the middle of the maze using a beaker. It will quickly try to shelter due to its aversion to the brightly lit environment. The mouse completes several sessions to test its capacity to find the refuge identified the first time again. After the learning phase, the position of the exit hole may be changed to evaluate the animal’s capacity to take on board a…

Photo
20170048_0045
Souris sur un labyrinthe de Barnes pour un test de mémoire spatiale complexe
20170048_0046
Open media modal

Mouse during an exploratory phase in a Barnes maze. The maze has 20 holes, with only one leading to a refuge. The mouse is first placed in the middle of the maze using a beaker. It will quickly try to shelter due to its aversion to the brightly lit environment. The mouse completes several sessions to test its capacity to find the refuge identified the first time again. After the learning phase, the position of the exit hole may be changed to evaluate the animal’s capacity to take on board a…

Photo
20170048_0046
Souris sur un labyrinthe de Barnes pour un test de mémoire spatiale complexe
20170048_0047
Open media modal

Mouse during an exploratory phase in a Barnes maze. The maze has 20 holes, with only one leading to a refuge. The mouse is first placed in the middle of the maze using a beaker. It will quickly try to shelter due to its aversion to the brightly lit environment. The mouse completes several sessions to test its capacity to find the refuge identified the first time again. After the learning phase, the position of the exit hole may be changed to evaluate the animal’s capacity to take on board a…

Photo
20170048_0047
Souris sur un labyrinthe de Barnes pour un test de mémoire spatiale complexe
20170048_0048
Open media modal

Mouse during an exploratory phase in a Barnes maze. The maze has 20 holes, with only one leading to a refuge. The mouse is first placed in the middle of the maze using a beaker. It will quickly try to shelter due to its aversion to the brightly lit environment. The mouse completes several sessions to test its capacity to find the refuge identified the first time again. After the learning phase, the position of the exit hole may be changed to evaluate the animal’s capacity to take on board a…

Photo
20170048_0048
Souris sur un labyrinthe de Barnes pour un test de mémoire spatiale complexe
20170048_0049
Open media modal

Social recognition test on mice. The mouse tested, referred to as the reference mouse, is placed in the middle of a system comprising three chambers linked by doors. A mouse familiar to the reference mouse is placed in a cage in a chamber at one end. Another mouse, not known by the reference mouse, is placed at the other end. A trajectory analysis device quantifies the frequency of interactions between the free mouse and each of the other two mice. The test is performed on mice with Alzheimer’s…

Photo
20170048_0049
Test de reconnaissance sociale chez des souris
20170048_0050
Open media modal

Social recognition test on mice. The mouse tested, referred to as the reference mouse, is placed in the middle of a system comprising three chambers linked by doors. A mouse familiar to the reference mouse is placed in a cage in a chamber at one end. Another mouse, not known by the reference mouse, is placed at the other end. A trajectory analysis device quantifies the frequency of interactions between the free mouse and each of the other two mice. The test is performed on mice with Alzheimer’s…

Photo
20170048_0050
Test de reconnaissance sociale chez des souris
20170048_0051
Open media modal

Social recognition test on mice. The mouse tested, referred to as the reference mouse, is placed in the middle of a system comprising three chambers linked by doors. A mouse familiar to the reference mouse is placed in a cage in a chamber at one end. Another mouse, not known by the reference mouse, is placed at the other end. A trajectory analysis device quantifies the frequency of interactions between the free mouse and each of the other two mice. The test is performed on mice with Alzheimer’s…

Photo
20170048_0051
Test de reconnaissance sociale chez des souris
20170048_0052
Open media modal

Social recognition test on mice. The mouse tested, referred to as the reference mouse, is placed in the middle of a system comprising three chambers linked by doors. A mouse familiar to the reference mouse is placed in a cage in a chamber at one end. Another mouse, not known by the reference mouse, is placed at the other end. A trajectory analysis device quantifies the frequency of interactions between the free mouse and each of the other two mice. The test is performed on mice with Alzheimer’s…

Photo
20170048_0052
Test de reconnaissance sociale chez des souris
20170048_0053
Open media modal

Social recognition test on mice. The mouse tested, referred to as the reference mouse, is placed in the middle of a system comprising three chambers linked by doors. A mouse familiar to the reference mouse is placed in a cage in a chamber at one end. Another mouse, not known by the reference mouse, is placed at the other end. A trajectory analysis device quantifies the frequency of interactions between the free mouse and each of the other two mice. The test is performed on mice with Alzheimer’s…

Photo
20170048_0053
Test de reconnaissance sociale chez des souris
20170048_0054
Open media modal

Social recognition test on mice. The mouse tested, referred to as the reference mouse, is placed in the middle of a system comprising three chambers linked by doors. A mouse familiar to the reference mouse is placed in a cage in a chamber at one end. Another mouse, not known by the reference mouse, is placed at the other end. A trajectory analysis device quantifies the frequency of interactions between the free mouse and each of the other two mice. The test is performed on mice with Alzheimer’s…

Photo
20170048_0054
Test de reconnaissance sociale chez des souris
20170048_0055
Open media modal

Social recognition test on mice. The mouse tested, referred to as the reference mouse, is placed in the middle of a system comprising three chambers linked by doors. A mouse familiar to the reference mouse is placed in a cage in a chamber at one end. Another mouse, not known by the reference mouse, is placed at the other end. A trajectory analysis device quantifies the frequency of interactions between the free mouse and each of the other two mice. The test is performed on mice with Alzheimer’s…

Photo
20170048_0055
Test de reconnaissance sociale chez des souris
20170048_0056
Open media modal

Feeding spiders before sociality tests. Spiders are generally solitary, but go through a temporary social period during their juvenile phase, which then ceases during the dispersion phase. Research scientists are trying to identify the fundamental mechanisms that favour the emergence of cooperation between individuals leading to the formation of lasting societies. To do this, they compare the behaviour of spiders found locally and in French Guiana, examining in particular how the availability…

Photo
20170048_0056
Nourrissage d'araignées en vue de tests de socialité
20170048_0057
Open media modal

Feeding spiders before sociality tests. Spiders are generally solitary, but go through a temporary social period during their juvenile phase, which then ceases during the dispersion phase. Research scientists are trying to identify the fundamental mechanisms that favour the emergence of cooperation between individuals leading to the formation of lasting societies. To do this, they compare the behaviour of spiders found locally and in French Guiana, examining in particular how the availability…

Photo
20170048_0057
Nourrissage d'araignées en vue de tests de socialité
20170048_0058
Open media modal

Feeding spiders before sociality tests. Spiders are generally solitary, but go through a temporary social period during their juvenile phase, which then ceases during the dispersion phase. Research scientists are trying to identify the fundamental mechanisms that favour the emergence of cooperation between individuals leading to the formation of lasting societies. To do this, they compare the behaviour of spiders found locally and in French Guiana, examining in particular how the availability…

Photo
20170048_0058
Nourrissage d'araignées en vue de tests de socialité
20170048_0059
Open media modal

Feeding spiders before sociality tests. Spiders are generally solitary, but go through a temporary social period during their juvenile phase, which then ceases during the dispersion phase. Research scientists are trying to identify the fundamental mechanisms that favour the emergence of cooperation between individuals leading to the formation of lasting societies. To do this, they compare the behaviour of spiders found locally and in French Guiana, examining in particular how the availability…

Photo
20170048_0059
Nourrissage d'araignées en vue de tests de socialité
20170048_0060
Open media modal

Feeding spiders before sociality tests. Spiders are generally solitary, but go through a temporary social period during their juvenile phase, which then ceases during the dispersion phase. Research scientists are trying to identify the fundamental mechanisms that favour the emergence of cooperation between individuals leading to the formation of lasting societies. To do this, they compare the behaviour of spiders found locally and in French Guiana, examining in particular how the availability…

Photo
20170048_0060
Nourrissage d'araignées en vue de tests de socialité
20170048_0061
Open media modal

Placing a juvenile spider in an arena for a sociality test. This spider will then be exposed to another young spider so that their interactions can be studied. Spiders are generally solitary, but go through a temporary social period during their juvenile phase, which then ceases during the dispersion phase. Research scientists are trying to identify the fundamental mechanisms that favour the emergence of cooperation between individuals leading to the formation of lasting societies.

Photo
20170048_0061
Dépôt d’une araignée au stade juvénile dans une arène pour un test de socialité
20170048_0062
Open media modal

Juvenile spider during a sociality test. Two young spiders are placed in this arena in order to study their interactions. Spiders are generally solitary, but go through a temporary social period during the juvenile phase prior to dispersion. Research scientists are trying to identify the fundamental mechanisms that favour the emergence of cooperation between individuals leading to the formation of lasting societies.

Photo
20170048_0062
Araignée de stade juvénile au cours d'un test de socialité
20170048_0063
Open media modal

Study of the behaviour of two juvenile spiders during a sociality test. Here, the research scientists are analysing their interactions and measuring the time that the individuals spend aggregated. Spiders are generally solitary, but go through a temporary social period during their juvenile phase, which then ceases during the dispersion phase. Research scientists are trying to identify the fundamental mechanisms that favour the emergence of cooperation between individuals leading to the…

Photo
20170048_0063
Etude des comportements de deux araignées juvéniles au cours d’un test de socialité
20170048_0064
Open media modal

Study of the behaviour of two juvenile spiders during a sociality test. Here, the research scientists are analysing their interactions and measuring the time that the individuals spend aggregated. Spiders are generally solitary, but go through a temporary social period during their juvenile phase, which then ceases during the dispersion phase. Research scientists are trying to identify the fundamental mechanisms that favour the emergence of cooperation between individuals leading to the…

Photo
20170048_0064
Etude des comportements de deux araignées juvéniles au cours d’un test de socialité
20170048_0065
Open media modal

Study of the behaviour of a spider faced with a lure. This device makes it possible to study the influence of cuticular compounds on interactions between spiders, which are generally solitary. These compounds could play a major role in the transition from the social phase of juveniles to the solitary phase of older individuals. Research scientists are trying to identify the fundamental mechanisms that favour the emergence of cooperation between individuals leading to the formation of lasting…

Photo
20170048_0065
Etude du comportement d'une araignée face à un leurre
20170048_0066
Open media modal

Study of the behaviour of a spider faced with a lure. This device makes it possible to study the influence of cuticular compounds on interactions between spiders, which are generally solitary. These compounds could play a major role in the transition from the social phase of juveniles to the solitary phase of older individuals. Research scientists are trying to identify the fundamental mechanisms that favour the emergence of cooperation between individuals leading to the formation of lasting…

Photo
20170048_0066
Etude du comportement d'une araignée face à un leurre
20170048_0067
Open media modal

Study of the behaviour of a spider faced with a lure. This device makes it possible to study the influence of cuticular compounds on interactions between spiders, which are generally solitary. These compounds could play a major role in the transition from the social phase of juveniles to the solitary phase of older individuals. Research scientists are trying to identify the fundamental mechanisms that favour the emergence of cooperation between individuals leading to the formation of lasting…

Photo
20170048_0067
Etude du comportement d'une araignée face à un leurre
20170048_0068
Open media modal

Study of the behaviour of a spider faced with a lure. This device makes it possible to study the influence of cuticular compounds on interactions between spiders, which are generally solitary. These compounds could play a major role in the transition from the social phase of juveniles to the solitary phase of older individuals. Research scientists are trying to identify the fundamental mechanisms that favour the emergence of cooperation between individuals leading to the formation of lasting…

Photo
20170048_0068
Etude du comportement d'une araignée face à un leurre
20170048_0069
Open media modal

Feeding spiders before sociality tests. Spiders are generally solitary, but go through a temporary social period during their juvenile phase, which then ceases during the dispersion phase. Research scientists are trying to identify the fundamental mechanisms that favour the emergence of cooperation between individuals leading to the formation of lasting societies. To do this, they compare the behaviour of spiders found locally and in French Guiana, examining in particular how the availability…

Photo
20170048_0069
Nourrissage d'araignées en vue de tests de socialité
20170048_0070
Open media modal

Feeding spiders before sociality tests. Spiders are generally solitary, but go through a temporary social period during their juvenile phase, which then ceases during the dispersion phase. Research scientists are trying to identify the fundamental mechanisms that favour the emergence of cooperation between individuals leading to the formation of lasting societies. To do this, they compare the behaviour of spiders found locally and in French Guiana, examining in particular how the availability…

Photo
20170048_0070
Nourrissage d'araignées en vue de tests de socialité
20170048_0071
Open media modal

Humidifying a vivarium containing local solitary spiders. Spiders are generally solitary, but go through a temporary social period during their juvenile phase, which then ceases during the dispersion phase. The spiders here undergo different tests so that their interactions can be analysed. These experiments make it possible to identify the fundamental mechanisms that favour the emergence of cooperation between individuals leading to the formation of lasting societies.

Photo
20170048_0071
Humidification d’un vivarium contenant des araignées solitaires locales
20170048_0072
Open media modal

Humidifying a vivarium containing local solitary spiders. Spiders are generally solitary, but go through a temporary social period during their juvenile phase, which then ceases during the dispersion phase. The spiders here undergo different tests so that their interactions can be analysed. These experiments make it possible to identify the fundamental mechanisms that favour the emergence of cooperation between individuals leading to the formation of lasting societies.

Photo
20170048_0072
Humidification d’un vivarium contenant des araignées solitaires locales
20170048_0073
Open media modal

Humidifying a vivarium containing local solitary spiders. Spiders are generally solitary, but go through a temporary social period during their juvenile phase, which then ceases during the dispersion phase. The spiders here undergo different tests so that their interactions can be analysed. These experiments make it possible to identify the fundamental mechanisms that favour the emergence of cooperation between individuals leading to the formation of lasting societies.

Photo
20170048_0073
Humidification d’un vivarium contenant des araignées solitaires locales
20170048_0074
Open media modal

Study of the behaviour of fungus-growing ants, Atta laevigata, in an experimental device reproducing their journeys along trails to the colony. Atta ants, South American ants also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. The ants known as "majors" or "soldiers" use their powerful jaws to cut the leaves into pieces, which are then transported by the medium-sized workers. The "minors" follow a chemical trail…

Photo
20170048_0074
Étude du comportement des fourmis champignonnistes,"Atta laevigata"
20170048_0075
Open media modal

Study of the behaviour of fungus-growing ants, Atta laevigata, in an experimental device reproducing their journeys along trails to the colony. Atta ants, South American ants also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. The ants known as "majors" or "soldiers" use their powerful jaws to cut the leaves into pieces, which are then transported by the medium-sized workers. The "minors" follow a chemical trail…

Photo
20170048_0075
Fourmis "Atta laevigata" acheminant des morceaux de feuille
20170048_0076
Open media modal

Study of the behaviour of fungus-growing ants, Atta laevigata, in an experimental device reproducing their journeys along trails to the colony. Atta ants, South American ants also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. The ants known as "majors" or "soldiers" use their powerful jaws to cut the leaves into pieces, which are then transported by the medium-sized workers. The "minors" follow a chemical trail…

Photo
20170048_0076
Fourmis "Atta laevigata" acheminant des morceaux de feuille
20170048_0077
Open media modal

Study of the behaviour of fungus-growing ants, Atta laevigata, in an experimental device reproducing their journeys along trails to the colony. Atta ants, South American ants also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. The ants known as "majors" or "soldiers" use their powerful jaws to cut the leaves into pieces, which are then transported by the medium-sized workers. The "minors" follow a chemical trail…

Photo
20170048_0077
Fourmis "Atta laevigata" acheminant des morceaux de feuille
20170048_0078
Open media modal

Study of the behaviour of fungus-growing ants, Atta laevigata, in an experimental device reproducing their journeys along trails to the colony. Atta ants, South American ants also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. The ants known as "majors" or "soldiers" use their powerful jaws to cut the leaves into pieces, which are then transported by the medium-sized workers. The "minors" follow a chemical trail…

Photo
20170048_0078
Fourmis "Atta laevigata" ouvrières acheminant des morceaux de feuille
20170048_0079
Open media modal

Ants of the species Atta laevigata cutting and gathering pieces of leaf placed in a box, in order to feed the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus. Atta ants, tropical ants from South America also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. The ants known as "majors" or "soldiers" use their powerful jaws to cut the leaves into pieces, which are then transported by the medium-sized workers. The "minors" follow a…

Photo
20170048_0079
Fourmis "Atta laevigata" ouvrières découpant et prélevant des morceaux de feuille
20170048_0080
Open media modal

Ants of the species Atta laevigata cutting and gathering pieces of leaf placed in a box, in order to feed the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus. Atta ants, tropical ants from South America also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. The ants known as "majors" or "soldiers" use their powerful jaws to cut the leaves into pieces, which are then transported by the medium-sized workers. The "minors" follow a…

Photo
20170048_0080
Fourmis "Atta laevigata" découpant des morceaux de feuille
20170048_0081
Open media modal

Ants of the species Atta laevigata cutting and gathering pieces of leaf placed in a box, in order to feed the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus. Atta ants, tropical ants from South America also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. The ants known as "majors" or "soldiers" use their powerful jaws to cut the leaves into pieces, which are then transported by the medium-sized workers. The "minors" follow a…

Photo
20170048_0081
Fourmis "Atta laevigata" découpant des morceaux de feuille
20170048_0082
Open media modal

Ants of the species Atta laevigata carrying pieces of leaf to feed the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus in an experimental device reproducing their journeys along trails to the colony. Atta ants, tropical ants from South America also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. The ants known as "majors" or "soldiers" use their powerful jaws to cut the leaves into pieces, which are then transported by the…

Photo
20170048_0082
Fourmis "Atta laevigata" acheminant des morceaux de feuille
20170048_0083
Open media modal

Ants of the species Atta laevigata carrying pieces of leaf to feed the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus. In this experimental device, they move into the nest built through a tube. Atta ants, tropical ants from South America also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. The ants known as "majors" or "soldiers" use their powerful jaws to cut the leaves into pieces, which are then transported by the medium…

Photo
20170048_0083
Fourmis "Atta laevigata" acheminant des morceaux de feuille
20170048_0084
Open media modal

Experimental device reproducing the structure of a colony of the fungus-growing ants Atta laevigata. The jars, linked via tubes, contain the various spaces of the colony, from the nest chamber hollowed out inside the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus to the dump where the anthill waste is deposited. Atta ants, tropical ants from South America also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. The ants known as …

Photo
20170048_0084
Dispostif expérimental reproduisant la structure d’une colonie de fourmis "Atta laevigata"
20170048_0085
Open media modal

Experimental device reproducing the structure of a colony of the fungus-growing ants Atta laevigata. The jars, linked via tubes, contain the various spaces of the colony, from the nest chamber hollowed out inside the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus to the dump where the anthill waste is deposited. Atta ants, tropical ants from South America also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. The ants known as …

Photo
20170048_0085
Dispostif expérimental reproduisant la structure d’une colonie de fourmis "Atta laevigata"
20170048_0086
Open media modal

Atta laevigata nest built inside the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus. The "minor" ants take the sections of leaf brought by the workers to feed the fungus. They then cut them up into smaller pieces and use them to cultivate the fungus, building their nest by hollowing out chambers inside it, and using it to feed their larvae. Atta ants, also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. This symbiosis is…

Photo
20170048_0086
Fourmilière d’"Atta laevigata" établie au sein du champignon “Leucoagaricus gongylophoru“
20170048_0087
Open media modal

Ants of the species Atta laevigata cutting and gathering pieces of leaf to feed the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus. Atta ants, tropical ants from South America also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. The ants known as "majors" or "soldiers" use their powerful jaws to cut the leaves into pieces, which are then transported by the medium-sized workers. The "minors" follow a chemical trail marked on the…

Photo
20170048_0087
Fourmis "Atta laevigata" découpant des morceaux de feuille
20170048_0088
Open media modal

Ants of the species Atta laevigata in the laboratory, travelling between their nest and a box containing vegetation. These ants cut and gather pieces of leaf to feed the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus. Atta ants, tropical ants from South America also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. The ants known as "majors" or "soldiers" use their powerful jaws to cut the leaves into pieces, which are then…

Photo
20170048_0088
Fourmis "Atta laevigata" acheminant des morceaux de feuille
20170048_0089
Open media modal

Ants of the species Atta laevigata cutting and gathering pieces of leaf to feed the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus. Atta ants, tropical ants from South America also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. The ants known as "majors" or "soldiers" use their powerful jaws to cut the leaves into pieces, which are then transported by the medium-sized workers. The "minors" follow a chemical trail marked on the…

Photo
20170048_0089
Fourmis "Atta laevigata" découpant des morceaux de feuille
20170048_0090
Open media modal

Ants of the species Atta laevigata carrying pieces of leaf to feed the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus in an experimental device reproducing their journeys along trails to the colony. Atta ants, tropical ants from South America also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. The ants known as "majors" or "soldiers" use their powerful jaws to cut the leaves into pieces, which are then transported by the…

Photo
20170048_0090
Fourmis "Atta laevigata" acheminant des morceaux de feuille
20170048_0091
Open media modal

Ants of the species Atta laevigata carrying pieces of leaf to feed the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus in an experimental device reproducing their journeys along trails to the colony. Atta ants, tropical ants from South America also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. The ants known as "majors" or "soldiers" use their powerful jaws to cut the leaves into pieces, which are then transported by the…

Photo
20170048_0091
Fourmis "Atta laevigata" acheminant des morceaux de feuille
20170048_0092
Open media modal

Ants of the species Atta laevigata carrying pieces of leaf to feed the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus in an experimental device reproducing their journeys along trails to the colony. Atta ants, tropical ants from South America also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. The ants known as "majors" or "soldiers" use their powerful jaws to cut the leaves into pieces, which are then transported by the…

Photo
20170048_0092
Fourmis "Atta laevigata" acheminant des morceaux de feuille
20170048_0093
Open media modal

Ants of the species Atta laevigata cutting and gathering pieces of leaf placed in a box to feed the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus. Atta ants, tropical ants from South America also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. The ants known as "majors" or "soldiers" use their powerful jaws to cut the leaves into pieces, which are then transported by the medium-sized workers. The "minors" follow a chemical…

Photo
20170048_0093
Fourmis "Atta laevigata" prélevant et découpant des morceaux de feuille
20170048_0094
Open media modal

Ants of the species Atta laevigata cutting and gathering pieces of leaf placed in a box to feed the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus. Atta ants, tropical ants from South America also known as leafcutter ants, live in symbiosis with the fungus Leucoagaricus gongylophorus, which they feed and feed from in turn. The ants known as "majors" or "soldiers" use their powerful jaws to cut the leaves into pieces, which are then transported by the medium-sized workers. The "minors" follow a chemical…

Photo
20170048_0094
Fourmis "Atta laevigata" prélevant et découpant des morceaux de feuille
20170048_0095
Open media modal

Apprentissage visuel discriminatif chez l'abeille testé en réalité virtuelle. Une abeille est fixée et placée sur un compensateur de locomotion constitué d'une boule en polystyrène posée sur un coussin d'air, ce qui permet à l'abeille de la bouger avec le seul mouvement de ses pattes. Ce système est dit en "closed-loop", cela signifie que chaque mouvement de l'abeille, enregistré grâce à deux capteurs de part et d'autres du compensateur, entraîne un mouvement de l'image à l'écran ; ce qui…

Photo
20170048_0095
Apprentissage visuel discriminatif chez l'abeille testé en réalité virtuelle
20170048_0096
Open media modal

Apprentissage visuel discriminatif chez l'abeille testé en réalité virtuelle. Une abeille est fixée et placée sur un compensateur de locomotion constitué d'une boule en polystyrène posée sur un coussin d'air, ce qui permet à l'abeille de la bouger avec le seul mouvement de ses pattes. Ce système est dit en "closed-loop", cela signifie que chaque mouvement de l'abeille, enregistré grâce à deux capteurs de part et d'autres du compensateur, entraîne un mouvement de l'image à l'écran ; ce qui…

Photo
20170048_0096
Apprentissage visuel discriminatif chez l'abeille testé en réalité virtuelle
20170048_0097
Open media modal

Apprentissage visuel discriminatif chez l'abeille testé en réalité virtuelle. Une abeille est fixée et placée sur un compensateur de locomotion constitué d'une boule en polystyrène posée sur un coussin d'air, ce qui permet à l'abeille de la bouger avec le seul mouvement de ses pattes. Ce système est dit en "closed-loop", cela signifie que chaque mouvement de l'abeille, enregistré grâce à deux capteurs de part et d'autres du compensateur, entraîne un mouvement de l'image à l'écran ; ce qui…

Photo
20170048_0097
Apprentissage visuel discriminatif chez l'abeille testé en réalité virtuelle
20170048_0098
Open media modal

Apprentissage visuel discriminatif chez l'abeille testé en réalité virtuelle. Une abeille est fixée et placée sur un compensateur de locomotion constitué d'une boule en polystyrène posée sur un coussin d'air, ce qui permet à l'abeille de la bouger avec le seul mouvement de ses pattes. Ce système est dit en "closed-loop", cela signifie que chaque mouvement de l'abeille, enregistré grâce à deux capteurs de part et d'autres du compensateur, entraîne un mouvement de l'image à l'écran ; ce qui…

Photo
20170048_0098
Apprentissage visuel discriminatif chez l'abeille testé en réalité virtuelle
20170048_0100
Open media modal

Apprentissage visuel discriminatif chez l'abeille testé en réalité virtuelle. Une abeille est fixée et placée sur un compensateur de locomotion constitué d'une boule en polystyrène posée sur un coussin d'air, ce qui permet à l'abeille de la bouger avec le seul mouvement de ses pattes. Ce système est dit en "closed-loop", cela signifie que chaque mouvement de l'abeille, enregistré grâce à deux capteurs de part et d'autres du compensateur, entraîne un mouvement de l'image à l'écran ; ce qui…

Photo
20170048_0100
Apprentissage visuel discriminatif chez l'abeille testé en réalité virtuelle
20170048_0101
Open media modal

Apprentissage visuel discriminatif chez l'abeille testé en réalité virtuelle. Une abeille est fixée et placée sur un compensateur de locomotion constitué d'une boule en polystyrène posée sur un coussin d'air, ce qui permet à l'abeille de la bouger avec le seul mouvement de ses pattes. Ce système est dit en "closed-loop", cela signifie que chaque mouvement de l'abeille, enregistré grâce à deux capteurs de part et d'autres du compensateur, entraîne un mouvement de l'image à l'écran ; ce qui…

Photo
20170048_0101
Apprentissage visuel discriminatif chez l'abeille testé en réalité virtuelle
20170048_0102
Open media modal

Appetite conditioning device for bees. The test is based on the observation that when their antennae come into contact with a sugary solution, bees react by extending their proboscis (mouthparts specialising in suction). Research scientists initially present a neutral odour together with the sugary solution, causing the bee to form an association. Once this has been learned, the bee will extend its proboscis on detecting the neutral odour alone. The experiment is filmed so that the bees’…

Photo
20170048_0102
Dispositif de conditionnement appétitif chez les abeilles
20170048_0103
Open media modal

Appetite conditioning device for bees. The test is based on the observation that when their antennae come into contact with a sugary solution, bees react by extending their proboscis (mouthparts specialising in suction). Research scientists initially present a neutral odour together with the sugary solution, causing the bee to form an association. Once this has been learned, the bee will extend its proboscis on detecting the neutral odour alone. The experiment is filmed so that the bees’…

Photo
20170048_0103
Dispositif de conditionnement appétitif chez les abeilles
20170048_0104
Open media modal

Appetite conditioning device for bees. The test is based on the observation that when their antennae come into contact with a sugary solution, bees react by extending their proboscis (mouthparts specialising in suction). Research scientists initially present a neutral odour together with the sugary solution, causing the bee to form an association. Once this has been learned, the bee will extend its proboscis on detecting the neutral odour alone. The experiment is filmed so that the bees’…

Photo
20170048_0104
Dispositif de conditionnement appétitif chez les abeilles
20170048_0105
Open media modal

Appetite conditioning device for bees. The test is based on the observation that when their antennae come into contact with a sugary solution, bees react by extending their proboscis (mouthparts specialising in suction). Research scientists initially present a neutral odour together with the sugary solution, causing the bee to form an association. Once this has been learned, the bee will extend its proboscis on detecting the neutral odour alone. The experiment is filmed so that the bees’…

Photo
20170048_0105
Dispositif de conditionnement appétitif chez les abeilles
20170048_0106
Open media modal

Appetite conditioning device for bees. The test is based on the observation that when their antennae come into contact with a sugary solution, bees react by extending their proboscis (mouthparts specialising in suction). Research scientists initially present a neutral odour together with the sugary solution, causing the bee to form an association. Once this has been learned, the bee will extend its proboscis on detecting the neutral odour alone. The experiment is filmed so that the bees’…

Photo
20170048_0106
Dispositif de conditionnement appétitif chez les abeilles
20170048_0107
Open media modal

Proboscis extension reflex (PER) appetite conditioning device for bees. When their antennae come into contact with a sugary solution, bees extend their proboscis (mouthparts specialising in suction). Research scientists initially present a neutral odour together with the sugary solution, causing the bee to form an association between the odour and the sugary reward. Here, the experimenter is rewarding the bee by presenting it with a toothpick soaked in sugar solution. Once this has been learned…

Photo
20170048_0107
Dispositif de conditionnement appétitif chez les abeilles
20170048_0108
Open media modal

Proboscis extension reflex (PER) appetite conditioning device for bees. When their antennae come into contact with a sugary solution, bees extend their proboscis (mouthparts specialising in suction). Research scientists initially present a neutral odour together with the sugary solution, causing the bee to form an association between the odour and the sugary reward. Here, the experimenter is rewarding the bee by presenting it with a toothpick soaked in sugar solution. Once this has been learned…

Photo
20170048_0108
Dispositif de conditionnement appétitif chez les abeilles
20170048_0109
Open media modal

Proboscis extension reflex (PER) appetite conditioning device for bees. When their antennae come into contact with a sugary solution, bees extend their proboscis (mouthparts specialising in suction). Research scientists initially present a neutral odour together with the sugary solution, causing the bee to form an association between the odour and the sugary reward. Here, the experimenter is rewarding the bee by presenting it with a toothpick soaked in sugar solution. Once this has been learned…

Photo
20170048_0109
Dispositif de conditionnement appétitif chez les abeilles
20170048_0110
Open media modal

Appetite conditioning device for bees. The test is based on the observation that when their antennae come into contact with a sugary solution, bees react by extending their proboscis (mouthparts specialising in suction). Research scientists initially present a neutral odour together with the sugary solution, causing the bee to form an association. Once this has been learned, the bee will extend its proboscis on detecting the neutral odour alone. The experiment is filmed so that the bees’…

Photo
20170048_0110
Dispositif de conditionnement appétitif chez les abeilles
20170048_0111
Open media modal

Proboscis extension reflex (PER) appetite conditioning device for bees. When their antennae come into contact with a sugary solution, bees extend their proboscis (mouthparts specialising in suction). Research scientists initially present a neutral odour together with the sugary solution, causing the bee to form an association between the odour and the sugary reward. Here, the experimenter is rewarding the bee by presenting it with a toothpick soaked in sugar solution. Once this has been learned…

Photo
20170048_0111
Dispositif de conditionnement appétitif chez les abeilles
20170048_0112
Open media modal

Proboscis extension reflex (PER) appetite conditioning device for bees. When their antennae come into contact with a sugary solution, bees extend their proboscis (mouthparts specialising in suction). Research scientists initially present a neutral odour together with the sugary solution, causing the bee to form an association between the odour and the sugary reward. Here, the experimenter is rewarding the bee by presenting it with a toothpick soaked in sugar solution. Once this has been learned…

Photo
20170048_0112
Dispositif de conditionnement appétitif chez les abeilles
20170048_0113
Open media modal

Proboscis extension reflex (PER) appetite conditioning device for bees. When their antennae come into contact with a sugary solution, bees extend their proboscis (mouthparts specialising in suction). Research scientists initially present a neutral odour together with the sugary solution, causing the bee to form an association between the odour and the sugary reward. Here, the experimenter is rewarding the bee by presenting it with a toothpick soaked in sugar solution. Once this has been learned…

Photo
20170048_0113
Dispositif de conditionnement appétitif chez les abeilles
20170048_0114
Open media modal

Proboscis extension reflex (PER) appetite conditioning device for bees. When their antennae come into contact with a sugary solution, bees extend their proboscis (mouthparts specialising in suction). Research scientists initially present a neutral odour together with the sugary solution, causing the bee to form an association between the odour and the sugary reward. Here, the experimenter is rewarding the bee by presenting it with a toothpick soaked in sugar solution. Once this has been learned…

Photo
20170048_0114
Dispositif de conditionnement appétitif chez les abeilles
20170048_0115
Open media modal

Proboscis extension reflex (PER) appetite conditioning device for bees. When their antennae come into contact with a sugary solution, bees extend their proboscis (mouthparts specialising in suction). Research scientists initially present a neutral odour together with the sugary solution, causing the bee to form an association between the odour and the sugary reward. Here, the experimenter is rewarding the bee by presenting it with a toothpick soaked in sugar solution. Once this has been learned…

Photo
20170048_0115
Dispositif de conditionnement appétitif chez les abeilles
20170048_0116
Open media modal

Phototaxis aversive conditioning device for bees. This device comprises two compartments illuminated independently by 38 RGB (red, green, blue) LEDs. Bees do not see red light. The experiment makes use of bees’ innate phototaxis (attraction to light). It involves attracting the bee from a red compartment (which is dark for the bee) to a compartment illuminated in blue or green. Video analysis software is used to record the position of the bee over time. When the bee enters the illuminated…

Photo
20170048_0116
Dispositif de conditionnement aversif testé chez les abeilles
20170048_0117
Open media modal

Phototaxis aversive conditioning device for bees. This device comprises two compartments illuminated independently by 38 RGB (red, green, blue) LEDs. Bees do not see red light. The experiment makes use of bees’ innate phototaxis (attraction to light). It involves attracting the bee from a red compartment (which is dark for the bee) to a compartment illuminated in blue or green. Video analysis software is used to record the position of the bee over time. When the bee enters the illuminated…

Photo
20170048_0117
Dispositif de conditionnement aversif testé chez les abeilles
20170048_0118
Open media modal

Phototaxis aversive conditioning device for bees. This device comprises two compartments illuminated independently by 38 RGB (red, green, blue) LEDs. Bees do not see red light. The experiment makes use of bees’ innate phototaxis (attraction to light). It involves attracting the bee from a red compartment (which is dark for the bee) to a compartment illuminated in blue or green. Video analysis software is used to record the position of the bee over time. When the bee enters the illuminated…

Photo
20170048_0118
Dispositif de conditionnement aversif testé chez les abeilles
20170048_0119
Open media modal

Phototaxis aversive conditioning device for bees. This device comprises two compartments illuminated independently by 38 RGB (red, green, blue) LEDs. Bees do not see red light. The experiment makes use of bees’ innate phototaxis (attraction to light). It involves attracting the bee from a red compartment (which is dark for the bee) to a compartment illuminated in blue or green. Video analysis software is used to record the position of the bee over time. When the bee enters the illuminated…

Photo
20170048_0119
Dispositif de conditionnement aversif testé chez les abeilles
20170048_0120
Open media modal

Phototaxis aversive conditioning device for bees. This device comprises two compartments illuminated independently by 38 RGB (red, green, blue) LEDs. Bees do not see red light. The experiment makes use of bees’ innate phototaxis (attraction to light). It involves attracting the bee from a red compartment (which is dark for the bee) to a compartment illuminated in blue or green. Video analysis software is used to record the position of the bee over time. When the bee enters the illuminated…

Photo
20170048_0120
Dispositif de conditionnement aversif testé chez les abeilles
20170048_0121
Open media modal

Phototaxis aversive conditioning device for bees. This device comprises two compartments illuminated independently by 38 RGB (red, green, blue) LEDs. Bees do not see red light. The experiment makes use of bees’ innate phototaxis (attraction to light). It involves attracting the bee from a red compartment (which is dark for the bee) to a compartment illuminated in blue or green. Video analysis software is used to record the position of the bee over time. When the bee enters the illuminated…

Photo
20170048_0121
Dispositif de conditionnement aversif testé chez les abeilles
20170048_0122
Open media modal

Phototaxis aversive conditioning device for bees. This device comprises two compartments illuminated independently by 38 RGB (red, green, blue) LEDs. Bees do not see red light. The experiment makes use of bees’ innate phototaxis (attraction to light). It involves attracting the bee from a red compartment (which is dark for the bee) to a compartment illuminated in blue or green. Video analysis software is used to record the position of the bee over time. When the bee enters the illuminated…

Photo
20170048_0122
Dispositif de conditionnement aversif testé chez les abeilles
20170048_0123
Open media modal

Phototaxis aversive conditioning device for bees. This device comprises two compartments illuminated independently by 38 RGB (red, green, blue) LEDs. Bees do not see red light. The experiment makes use of bees’ innate phototaxis (attraction to light). It involves attracting the bee from a red compartment (which is dark for the bee) to a compartment illuminated in blue or green. Video analysis software is used to record the position of the bee over time. When the bee enters the illuminated…

Photo
20170048_0123
Dispositif de conditionnement aversif testé chez les abeilles
20170048_0124
Open media modal

Phototaxis aversive conditioning device for bees. This device comprises two compartments illuminated independently by 38 RGB (red, green, blue) LEDs. Bees do not see red light. The experiment makes use of bees’ innate phototaxis (attraction to light). It involves attracting the bee from a red compartment (which is dark for the bee) to a compartment illuminated in blue or green. Video analysis software is used to record the position of the bee over time. When the bee enters the illuminated…

Photo
20170048_0124
Dispositif de conditionnement aversif testé chez les abeilles
20170048_0125
Open media modal

Preparation of fruit flies for conditioning followed by a memory test. For conditioning, the fruit flies are placed in a cylinder with six holes, with 40 flies per hole. First, they are exposed to an odour together with an immediate electric shock. They are then exposed to a different odour without any shock. This cycle may be repeated several times to engage their long-term memory. The third phase, which takes place on another device, involves placing the flies between the first odour, on one…

Photo
20170048_0125
Préparation de drosophiles avant de les soumettre à un conditionnement en vue d'un test de mémoire
20170048_0126
Open media modal

Preparation of fruit flies for conditioning followed by a memory test. For conditioning, the fruit flies are placed in a cylinder with six holes, with 40 flies per hole. First, they are exposed to an odour together with an immediate electric shock. They are then exposed to a different odour without any shock. This cycle may be repeated several times to engage their long-term memory. The third phase, which takes place on another device, involves placing the flies between the first odour, on one…

Photo
20170048_0126
Préparation de drosophiles avant de les soumettre à un conditionnement en vue d'un test de mémoire
20170048_0127
Open media modal

Preparation of fruit flies for conditioning followed by a memory test. For conditioning, the fruit flies are placed in a cylinder with six holes, with 40 flies per hole. First, they are exposed to an odour together with an immediate electric shock. They are then exposed to a different odour without any shock. This cycle may be repeated several times to engage their long-term memory. The third phase, which takes place on another device, involves placing the flies between the first odour, on one…

Photo
20170048_0127
Préparation de drosophiles avant de les soumettre à un conditionnement en vue d'un test de mémoire
20170048_0128
Open media modal

Preparation of fruit flies for conditioning followed by a memory test. For conditioning, the fruit flies are placed in a cylinder with six holes, with 40 flies per hole. First, they are exposed to an odour together with an immediate electric shock. They are then exposed to a different odour without any shock. This cycle may be repeated several times to engage their long-term memory. The third phase, which takes place on another device, involves placing the flies between the first odour, on one…

Photo
20170048_0128
Préparation de drosophiles avant de les soumettre à un conditionnement en vue d'un test de mémoire
20170048_0129
Open media modal

Preparation of fruit flies for conditioning followed by a memory test. For conditioning, the fruit flies are placed in a cylinder with six holes, with 40 flies per hole. First, they are exposed to an odour together with an immediate electric shock. They are then exposed to a different odour without any shock. This cycle may be repeated several times to engage their long-term memory. The third phase, which takes place on another device, involves placing the flies between the first odour, on one…

Photo
20170048_0129
Préparation de drosophiles avant de les soumettre à un conditionnement en vue d'un test de mémoire
20170048_0130
Open media modal

Preparation of fruit flies for conditioning followed by a memory test. For conditioning, the fruit flies are placed in a cylinder with six holes, with 40 flies per hole. First, they are exposed to an odour together with an immediate electric shock. They are then exposed to a different odour without any shock. This cycle may be repeated several times to engage their long-term memory. The third phase, which takes place on another device, involves placing the flies between the first odour, on one…

Photo
20170048_0130
Préparation de drosophiles avant de les soumettre à un conditionnement en vue d'un test de mémoire
20170048_0131
Open media modal

Cylinder containing 240 fruit flies for conditioning before a memory test. First, they are exposed to an odour together with an immediate electric shock. They are then exposed to a different odour without any shock. This cycle may be repeated several times to engage their long-term memory. The third phase, which takes place on another device, involves placing the flies between the first odour, on one side, and the second odour, on the other. It seems that the number of conditioned fruit flies…

Photo
20170048_0131
Barillet contenant 240 drosophiles soumises à un conditionnement en vue d'un test de mémoire
20170048_0132
Open media modal

Preparation of fruit flies for conditioning followed by a memory test. For conditioning, the fruit flies are placed in a cylinder with six holes, with 40 flies per hole. First, they are exposed to an odour together with an immediate electric shock. They are then exposed to a different odour without any shock. This cycle may be repeated several times to engage their long-term memory. The third phase, which takes place on another device, involves placing the flies between the first odour, on one…

Photo
20170048_0132
Préparation de drosophiles avant de les soumettre à un conditionnement en vue d'un test de mémoire
20170048_0133
Open media modal

Placing cylinders containing fruit flies into a conditioning device. First, they are exposed to an odour together with an immediate electric shock. They are then exposed to a different odour without any shock. This cycle may be repeated several times to engage their long-term memory. The third phase, which takes place on another device, involves placing the flies between the first odour, on one side, and the second odour, on the other. It seems that the number of conditioned fruit flies that…

Photo
20170048_0133
Intégration de barillets contenant des drosophiles dans un dispositif de conditionnement
20170048_0134
Open media modal

Placing cylinders containing fruit flies into a conditioning device. First, they are exposed to an odour together with an immediate electric shock. They are then exposed to a different odour without any shock. This cycle may be repeated several times to engage their long-term memory. The third phase, which takes place on another device, involves placing the flies between the first odour, on one side, and the second odour, on the other. It seems that the number of conditioned fruit flies that…

Photo
20170048_0134
Intégration de barillets contenant des drosophiles dans un dispositif de conditionnement
20170048_0135
Open media modal

Placing cylinders containing fruit flies into a conditioning device. First, they are exposed to an odour together with an immediate electric shock. They are then exposed to a different odour without any shock. This cycle may be repeated several times to engage their long-term memory. The third phase, which takes place on another device, involves placing the flies between the first odour, on one side, and the second odour, on the other. It seems that the number of conditioned fruit flies that…

Photo
20170048_0135
Intégration de barillets contenant des drosophiles dans un dispositif de conditionnement
20170048_0136
Open media modal

Device to condition fruit flies before a memory test. Three cylinders, each containing 240 fruit flies in six tubes, are fastened to this device. First, the flies are exposed to an odour together with an immediate electric shock. They are then exposed to a different odour without any shock. This cycle may be repeated several times to engage their long-term memory. The third phase, which takes place on another device, involves placing the flies between the first odour, on one side, and the…

Photo
20170048_0136
Dispositif de conditionnement de drosophiles, avant un test de mémoire
20170048_0137
Open media modal

Retrieving fruit flies after conditioning for a memory test. For this test, the fruit flies are placed in a cylinder with six holes, with 40 flies per hole. First, they are exposed to an odour together with an immediate electric shock. They are then exposed to a different odour without any shock. This cycle is repeated several times to engage their long-term memory. The third phase, which takes place on another device, involves placing the flies between the first odour, on one side, and the…

Photo
20170048_0137
Récupération de drosophiles après conditionnement en vue d’un test de mémoire
20170048_0138
Open media modal

Retrieving fruit flies after conditioning for a memory test. For this test, the fruit flies are placed in a cylinder with six holes, with 40 flies per hole. First, they are exposed to an odour together with an immediate electric shock. They are then exposed to a different odour without any shock. This cycle is repeated several times to engage their long-term memory. The third phase, which takes place on another device, involves placing the flies between the first odour, on one side, and the…

Photo
20170048_0138
Récupération de drosophiles après conditionnement en vue d’un test de mémoire
20170048_0139
Open media modal

Retrieving fruit flies after conditioning for a memory test. For this test, the fruit flies are placed in a cylinder with six holes, with 40 flies per hole. First, they are exposed to an odour together with an immediate electric shock. They are then exposed to a different odour without any shock. This cycle is repeated several times to engage their long-term memory. The third phase, which takes place on another device, involves placing the flies between the first odour, on one side, and the…

Photo
20170048_0139
Récupération de drosophiles après conditionnement en vue d’un test de mémoire
20170048_0140
Open media modal

Retrieving fruit flies after conditioning for a memory test. For this test, the fruit flies are placed in a cylinder with six holes, with 40 flies per hole. First, they are exposed to an odour together with an immediate electric shock. They are then exposed to a different odour without any shock. This cycle is repeated several times to engage their long-term memory. The third phase, which takes place on another device, involves placing the flies between the first odour, on one side, and the…

Photo
20170048_0140
Récupération de drosophiles après conditionnement en vue d’un test de mémoire
20170048_0141
Open media modal

Tubes containing fruit flies for conditioning before a memory test. For this test, the fruit flies are placed in a cylinder with six holes, with 40 flies per hole. First, they are exposed to an odour together with an immediate electric shock. They are then exposed to a different odour without any shock. This cycle is repeated several times to engage their long-term memory. The third phase involves placing the flies between the first odour, on one side, and the second odour, on the other side…

Photo
20170048_0141
Tubes contenant des drosophiles utilisées pour un conditionnement en vue d'un test de mémoire
20170048_0142
Open media modal

Placing fruit flies in a device to test learning or memory. Preconditioned flies have the option of moving either towards an odour that was delivered with an electric shock during conditioning, on one side, or towards an odour not associated with an electric shock, on the other. The results show that the number of these flies that move towards the second odour (without a shock) is 30-40% higher in comparison to so-called "naive" (unconditioned) fruit flies. Other parameters can also be analysed…

Photo
20170048_0142
Intégration de drosophiles dans un dispositif de test de l’apprentissage ou de la mémoire
20170048_0143
Open media modal

Placing fruit flies in a device to test learning or memory. Preconditioned flies have the option of moving either towards an odour that was delivered with an electric shock during conditioning, on one side, or towards an odour not associated with an electric shock, on the other. The results show that the number of these flies that move towards the second odour (without a shock) is 30-40% higher in comparison to so-called "naive" (unconditioned) fruit flies. Other parameters can also be analysed…

Photo
20170048_0143
Tube contenant des drosophiles utilisées pour un conditionnement en vue d'un test de mémoire
20170048_0144
Open media modal

Placing fruit flies in a device to test memory. Preconditioned flies have the option of moving either towards an odour that was delivered with an electric shock during conditioning, on one side, or towards an odour not associated with an electric shock, on the other. The results show that the number of these flies that move towards the second odour (without a shock) is 30-40% higher in comparison to so-called "naive" (unconditioned) fruit flies. Other parameters can also be analysed, such as…

Photo
20170048_0144
Intégration de drosophiles dans un dispositif de test de l’apprentissage ou de la mémoire
20170048_0145
Open media modal

Fruit fly learning or memory test. Preconditioned flies have the option of moving either towards an odour that was delivered with an electric shock during conditioning, on one side, or towards an odour not associated with an electric shock, on the other. The results show that the number of these flies that move towards the second odour (without a shock) is 30-40% higher in comparison to so-called "naive" (unconditioned) fruit flies. Other parameters can also be analysed, such as how social…

Photo
20170048_0145
Test d’apprentissage ou de mémoire sur des drosophiles
20170048_0146
Open media modal

Fruit fly learning or memory test. Preconditioned flies have the option of moving either towards an odour that was delivered with an electric shock during conditioning, on one side, or towards an odour not associated with an electric shock, on the other. The results show that the number of these flies that move towards the second odour (without a shock) is 30-40% higher in comparison to so-called "naive" (unconditioned) fruit flies. Other parameters can also be analysed, such as how social…

Photo
20170048_0146
Test d’apprentissage ou de mémoire sur des drosophiles
20170048_0147
Open media modal

Fruit fly learning or memory test. Preconditioned flies have the option of moving either towards an odour that was delivered with an electric shock during conditioning, on one side, or towards an odour not associated with an electric shock, on the other. The results show that the number of these flies that move towards the second odour (without a shock) is 30-40% higher in comparison to so-called "naive" (unconditioned) fruit flies. Other parameters can also be analysed, such as how social…

Photo
20170048_0147
Test d’apprentissage ou de mémoire sur des drosophiles
20170048_0148
Open media modal

Fruit fly learning or memory test. Preconditioned flies have the option of moving either towards an odour that was delivered with an electric shock during conditioning, on one side, or towards an odour not associated with an electric shock, on the other. The results show that the number of these flies that move towards the second odour (without a shock) is 30-40% higher in comparison to so-called "naive" (unconditioned) fruit flies. Other parameters can also be analysed, such as how social…

Photo
20170048_0148
Test d’apprentissage ou de mémoire sur des drosophiles
20170048_0149
Open media modal

Handling the social amoeba Physarum polycephalum, commonly referred to as the "blob". The many characteristics of this primitive, unicellular organism include the ability to heal itself in less than two minutes and reorganise its entire venous system in three hours. It can also be cloned if it is cut into 1-mm pieces. Sensitive to light, it grows in damp environments and feeds on bacteria and fungi. It can double in size each day. Although it has no brain, it is capable of learning from its…

Photo
20170048_0149
Manipulation du myxomycète, "Physarum polycephalum"
20170048_0150
Open media modal

Cutting up the social amoeba Physarum polycephalum, commonly referred to as the "blob". The many characteristics of this primitive, unicellular organism include the ability to heal itself in less than two minutes and reorganise its entire venous system in three hours. It can also be cloned if it is cut into 1-mm pieces. Sensitive to light, it grows in damp environments and feeds on bacteria and fungi. It can double in size each day. Although it has no brain, it is capable of learning from its…

Photo
20170048_0150
Découpage du myxomycète, "Physarum polycephalum"
20170048_0151
Open media modal

Handling the social amoeba Physarum polycephalum, commonly referred to as the "blob", after it has been cut in two. The many characteristics of this primitive, unicellular organism include the ability to heal itself in less than two minutes and reorganise its entire venous system in three hours. It can also be cloned if it is cut into 1-mm pieces. Sensitive to light, it grows in damp environments and feeds on bacteria and fungi. It can double in size each day. Although it has no brain, it is…

Photo
20170048_0151
Manipulation du myxomycète, "Physarum polycephalum", coupé en deux
20170048_0152
Open media modal

Watering a Petri dish intended for the social amoeba Physarum polycephalum, commonly referred to as the "blob". This primitive, unicellular organism grows in damp environments and feeds on bacteria and fungi, which it digests using chemical substances. It can double in size each day. If it dries out, it forms a sclerotium (the overwintering form of fungi) and eliminates accumulated toxicity. Just a few drops of water are all it takes to revive it. It is also sensitive to light, and is kept in…

Photo
20170048_0152
Humidification d’une boite de Pétri accueillant le myxomycète, "Physarum polycephalum"
20170048_0153
Open media modal

Placing the social amoeba Physarum polycephalum, commonly referred to as the "blob", in a damp Petri dish. This primitive, unicellular organism grows in damp environments and feeds on bacteria and fungi, which it digests using chemical substances. It can double in size each day. If it dries out, it forms a sclerotium (the overwintering form of fungi) and eliminates accumulated toxicity. Just a few drops of water are all it takes to revive it. It is also sensitive to light, and is kept in an…

Photo
20170048_0153
Dépôt du myxomycète, "Physarum polycephalum", dans une boîte de Petri humidifiée
20170048_0154
Open media modal

Feeding the social amoeba Physarum polycephalum, commonly referred to as the "blob", with rolled oats. This primitive, unicellular organism grows in damp environments and feeds on bacteria and fungi, which it digests using chemical substances. In the laboratory, research scientists feed it with rolled oats. If it is fed correctly, it can double in size each day and grow to measure up to 10 m2. If it dries out, it forms a sclerotium (the overwintering form of fungi) and eliminates accumulated…

Photo
20170048_0154
Nourrissage du myxomycète, "Physarum polycephalum" avec des flocons d’avoine
20170048_0155
Open media modal

Feeding the social amoeba Physarum polycephalum, commonly referred to as the "blob", with rolled oats. This primitive, unicellular organism grows in damp environments and feeds on bacteria and fungi, which it digests using chemical substances. In the laboratory, research scientists feed it with rolled oats. If it is fed correctly, it can double in size each day and grow to measure up to 10 m2. If it dries out, it forms a sclerotium (the overwintering form of fungi) and eliminates accumulated…

Photo
20170048_0155
Nourrissage du myxomycète, "Physarum polycephalum" avec des flocons d’avoine
20170048_0156
Open media modal

Watering the social amoeba Physarum polycephalum, commonly referred to as the "blob", which has been fed with rolled oats. This primitive, unicellular organism grows in damp environments and feeds on bacteria and fungi, which it digests using chemical substances. In the laboratory, research scientists feed it with rolled oats. It can double in size each day. If it dries out, it forms a sclerotium (the overwintering form of fungi) and eliminates accumulated toxicity. Just a few drops of water…

Photo
20170048_0156
Humidification du myxomycète, "Physarum polycephalum", nourri aux flocons d’avoine
20170048_0157
Open media modal

Placing the social amoeba Physarum polycephalum, commonly referred to as the "blob", in an incubator. Kept in Petri dishes, this primitive, unicellular organism is fed and watered to begin with. The blob can double in size each day, and moves by extending its pseudopodia while contracting its membrane and venous system. In the laboratory, research scientists keep it in an incubator as it does not tolerate much light and needs to be kept damp. This also lets them observe its development via an…

Photo
20170048_0157
Dépôt du myxomycète, "Physarum polycephalum", dans une étuve
20170048_0158
Open media modal

Observation of embryonic cortical neurons from the brain of a rat using a fluorescence microscope. Research scientists observe the mitochondria (cell energy centres) to determine their number, size and distribution in the various neuronal compartments (dendrites, axons and synapses). The mitochondria form a more or less interconnected network. Their size varies depending on the neuronal compartment and the physiological and pathological context. For example, in dominant optic atrophy, a…

Photo
20170048_0158
Observation de neurones de cortex embryonnaire de cerveau de rat en microscopie de fluorescence
20170048_0159
Open media modal

Observation of embryonic cortical neurons from the brain of a rat using a fluorescence microscope. Research scientists observe the mitochondria (cell energy centres) to determine their number, size and distribution in the various neuronal compartments (dendrites, axons and synapses). The mitochondria form a more or less interconnected network. Their size varies depending on the neuronal compartment and the physiological and pathological context. For example, in dominant optic atrophy, a…

Photo
20170048_0159
Observation de neurones de cortex embryonnaire de cerveau de rat en microscopie de fluorescence
20170048_0160
Open media modal

Observation of embryonic cortical neurons from the brain of a rat using a fluorescence microscope. Research scientists observe the mitochondria (cell energy centres) to determine their number, size and distribution in the various neuronal compartments (dendrites, axons and synapses). The mitochondria form a more or less interconnected network. Their size varies depending on the neuronal compartment and the physiological and pathological context. For example, in dominant optic atrophy, a…

Photo
20170048_0160
Observation de neurones de cortex embryonnaire de cerveau de rat en microscopie de fluorescence

Scientific topics

CNRS Images,

Our work is guided by the way scientists question the world around them and we translate their research into images to help people to understand the world better and to awaken their curiosity and wonderment.